In
my previous post When Rhine and Pearce Got Smoking Gun Evidence
for ESP, I discussed a series of tests that provide very good
evidence for ESP (extrasensory perception). In 2013 this evidence and
other evidence for ESP was attacked in a book by Brian Clegg entitled
Extra Sensory. Although Clegg claims in the last paragraph of
this book that he was examining the topic of ESP “with an open
mind,” his complete hostility toward the topic is made clear
throughout his very jaundiced book, which is very much a one-sided
tract. Let's take a look at Clegg's attack, and see whether it has
any weight.
First,
let me describe the experiments with Hubert E. Pearce Jr., which were
performed at the prestigious Duke University during the 1930's, under
the supervision of Professor Joseph Rhine. The main series of tests
were tests not of telepathy (also known as mind-reading), but tests
of clairvoyance, an ability of a single mind to obtain information
through non-sensory means. Both telepathy and clairvoyance fall under
the category of ESP or extrasensory perception, since they would seem
to involve some kind of anomalous perception that does not involve
the senses.
The
clairvoyance tests done with Pearce (described here) were simple. Two people
would sit at a table, the person being tested, and an observer.
Twelve decks of cards were put on the table, cards known as Zener
cards, that can have one of 5 different symbols on one side (with all
the cards having the same appearance on the opposite side).
The person being tested would shuffle the cards, and the observer would then cut the cards, assuring that the top cards were random. The person being tested would then hold a series of cards (either 5 or 25) one by one, face down, and guess which of the five symbols was on the unseen side of the card. After the series was done there would then be a stack of either 5 or 25 cards which had been the target of guesses. Those cards would then be turned face up one by one. As each card was turned face up, the observer would record whether the previous guess about that card's symbol was correct or not correct.
Zener cards used in ESP testing
The person being tested would shuffle the cards, and the observer would then cut the cards, assuring that the top cards were random. The person being tested would then hold a series of cards (either 5 or 25) one by one, face down, and guess which of the five symbols was on the unseen side of the card. After the series was done there would then be a stack of either 5 or 25 cards which had been the target of guesses. Those cards would then be turned face up one by one. As each card was turned face up, the observer would record whether the previous guess about that card's symbol was correct or not correct.
With
a variety of observers and with a variety of testing conditions (some
of which would have absolutely precluded the possibility of
cheating), Hubert Pearce got astonishing results too good to be
explained by chance. Below is the result of these tests. These are tests in which the expected chance result (average per 25) is only 5.
Pearce
got 3746 correct guesses out of 10,300. We can use the very handy
binomial probability calculator at this
site to calculate the likelihood of these results. The calculator
gives a probability of simply 0 when we type in the overall results,
so let's use a subset to try to get some non-zero result. Let's use
only rows 2 and 6, involving either Pearce looking away from the
cards or a screen between Pearce and the cards, either one of which
should have ruled out any possibility of cheating. When I type these
results in the binomial probability calculator, I get a result with a
chance probability of 5 chances in 100 trillion, which we can round
to be 1 chance in 10 trillion. This is a result we should never
expect to get by pure chance even if we tested with every single
person in the human race.
Now
what does Clegg say to try to discredit this very convincing evidence
for ESP? For one thing, he suggests that Pearce may have got his
result by looking at cards seen in someone's glasses. This is a
completely illegitimate explanation for any of the results involving
Pearce. None of the results (neither the results discussed above, nor
the equally dramatic Pearce-Pratt experiment described below)
involved one person holding cards while looking at them while someone
nearby tried to guess the cards.
Pearce
was never trained in magic, and was never caught cheating during any
of the long series of tests he was involved which, which dragged on for
months. But this does not stop Clegg from offering the suggestion
that Pearce's incredible results were the result of cheating by him.
Clegg states, “I can think of three or four ways to do this, the
most obvious being to sneak a peek at an upcoming card when the
observer was concentrating on recording the previous guess.”
Clegg's
absurd reasoning here is wholly without merit. If one person is
sitting directly across a table from another person, and the second
person is taking a second to write down which of five guesses was
made (which almost certainly would take no more than a few seconds, since
it can be done by a one-letter notation), the first person would have
a very high chance of being detected any particular time that he
tried to peek at the top value of a card deck lying on the table.
This is because humans have peripheral vision. If you are writing a
one-letter jot on a piece of paper on a table, you can certainly see
in the “corner of your eye” with your peripheral vision someone
reaching out his hand to peek at the top card in a deck. I may also note that the
type of cards used here (Zener cards) don't even have an indication
of their value on their corner (unlike playing cards). So you can't
peek at the value of a Zener card just by turning its corner – you
have to flip the card over or half-turn the card.
But
in order for Pearce to have got the results that Rhine recorded, he
would have had to peek at the value of the cards hundreds of
times without ever being detected once by an observer sitting right
across from him (and have this happen with a number of different
observers). Under such conditions there is no chance that someone
would be able to successfully peek at the top value more than about
10 times without being detected, and it is very likely that the first
such attempt would be detected.
Another
reason why Clegg's reasoning is completely without substance is that
the table above shows that Pearce got extremely compelling results
even in 3 series in which there was zero chance of peeking, one in
which he was forced to look away from the observer, and two others in
which there was a screen between the cards and Pearce's face. In the
two “looking away” series (series 2 and 3 above) he got a total 515 successes out of 1125 (a result which there is basically zero chance of you getting by mere chance). In the two “screen between the cards and the card
guesser” tests the results were 215 correct guesses out of 600,
which has a probability of less than 1 in 10 trillion (1 in
10,000,000,000,000) according to the binomial calculator I just linked to.
Clegg
also suggests (without any evidence) that Pearce may have somehow made markings on the cards
allowing him to cheat. This extremely implausible suggestion is also without merit, as
Rhine's original account of the tests states, “We brought in new
cards many times, without there resulting any change in the level of
scoring.” The fact that 12 decks of cards were on the table for the
tests also means that card markings cannot explain the results. Clegg
also suggests that Pearce “had the opportunity to take a quick look
at the whole pack while not being observed.” This suggestion is
also without merit because the protocol involved cards that were
shuffled and cut before anyone guessed about their value.
Having
discussed the Pearce-Rhine clairvoyance tests, and having offered no
objections with any substance, Clegg goes on to discuss the equally
compelling Pratt-Pearce tests on telepathy. The format for these
tests was very different. A person called the observer (Professor
Pratt) shuffled a deck of Zener cards, and at particular intervals
would turn the top card and record its value. The person being tested
(Pearce) was in distant location 100 yards away or even farther
away. The person being tested had instructions to begin guessing
which card had been turned, beginning at a particular time known to
both the observer and the person being tested. After a particular
length of time had passed, both the observer and the person being
tested would hand in their written results to a third person, who
would check and see how many of the guesses were accurate.
The tests (described here) were run from August 1933 to March 1934, totaling 74 runs
through a pack of 25 cards. The rate of card guessing was 1 per
minute. The total amount of time that Pearce spent guessing cards was
about 30 hours. 1850
cards were dealt, and the expected chance success rate was about 370
cards. Instead, Pearce got 558 correct guesses.
The
results of the test are shown below. The final column P shows the
probability of getting these result by chance, which was about 1 in
1022 or 1 in 10,000,000,000,000,000,000,000.
To
try to debunk this result, Clegg suggests the idea that Pearce
secretly sneaked over to Pratt's office, and then peeked over a crack
at the top of the door. He restates an old idea that Pearce may have
peeked through a transom at the top of Pratt's office. This
suggestion is not at all credible for several reasons. For one thing,
there's a 99% likelihood that no one would ever attempt so risky a
procedure. Then there's the 99% likelihood that if someone did
attempt such a procedure, he would have been detected by Pratt (or by
Rhine, who joined Pratt for one of the series). If you are close
enough to see the values of cards being turned by peeking through a
transom, you are close enough to be heard or seen by the person
turning those cards (partially because standing on a chair for long
periods inevitably results in chair squeaks that can be heard). Then
there's the fact that if you were to spend hours peeking through a
transom at the top of a door (which would require standing on a
chair), there's a 99.9% likelihood that you would be detected by some
passerby, who would either report this suspicious behavior to the
professor, or ask aloud “why are you peeking through that transom,”
which would give you away (the halls of universities tend to have
lots of foot traffic at all hours of the day). So this transom
objection is not a reasonable reason for rejecting the Pearce-Pratt
result as being convincing evidence for ESP.
The
dismal failure of Clegg's debunking efforts can be shown as follows:
suppose we accept both this enormously implausible “transom
peeking” idea and also the infinitely more implausible idea of
hundreds of card deck peeks going unnoticed by an observer directly
across the table (which is rather like assuming that someone can
really jump to the moon). Then you still have not explained
away the evidence for ESP involving tests with Pearce – for even
then you still have no explanations for the extremely compelling
evidence produced when there was a screen between the card guesser
and the cards (series 6 listed in the first table) – tests in which
there were 215 correct guesses out of 600, which has a probability of
less than 1 in 10 trillion (1 in 10,000,000,000,000).
The
evidence discussed here involving Hubert Pearce is only one piece of
a great mountain of evidence showing the reality of ESP. Another
very important piece is the ganzfeld studies done in recent decades.
Repeated many times by many experimenters, these studies have
consistently shown that subjects undergoing sensory deprivation are
able to score at a rate of between 30% and 32% on tests in which the
chance result should only be 25%. This is overwhelming evidence for
ESP.
Clegg
is not able to debunk this evidence. His approach is basically to
look for anything at all which he regards as a discrepancy between
the way he would ideally do such a test, and the way the test was done, and to call that an error. The same type of reasoning might
lead someone to say that your SAT score is not reliable because there
was the “error” that there were not steel walls between yourself
and the test takers to your right and left.
Clegg
concentrates on mentioning issues with the early versions of the
ganzfeld experients. But the designers of these experiments closely
worked with skeptics, and came up with a joint statement agreeing on
protocols that should be followed henceforward to assure the
reliability of these experiments. Even after these tightened-up
protocols were put in place, the experiments continued to get results
showing dramatic evidence of ESP, with the subjects achieving
success rates between 30% and 32% far in excess of the 25% success
rate they should be getting if ESP does not exist. As wikipedia.com
reports, “In 2010, Lance Storm, Patrizio Tressoldi, and Lorenzo Di
Risio analyzed 29 ganzfeld studies from 1997 to 2008. Of the 1,498
trials, 483 produced hits, corresponding to a hit rate of 32.2%.”
That is a rate far above the expected chance rate of 25%. See the pdf here for a meta analysis reporting the 30% number.
Contrary
to the impression Clegg tries to create in his 2013 book (with some
blatant cherry-picking that omits results after the year 2000), the
spectacular success of the ganzfeld experiments have continued even
after the procedures had their protocols tightened-up. These recent
tests between 1997 and 2014 provide very good evidence for ESP, as
the likelihood of you getting these results by chance is much less
than 1 in a trillion. Why is it that Clegg takes five pages to
discuss the ganzfeld experiments, without taking a sentence to state
the results they got (a sentence like this: the chance expectation
was 25%, but the results were between 30 and 32%)? It's kind of like
someone arguing that Ty Cobb was a poor hitter, and conveniently
failing to mention Cobb's batting average.
The
evidence for ESP is overwhelming, and continues to accumulate. This
evidence holds up very well to Clegg's unsound assault, which
consists mainly of the noise of blank shells firing. Besides abundant
anecdotal evidence that frequently crops up in human experience (such
as when we think of someone who rarely calls on the phone, just
before he calls on the phone), there is over 100 years of compelling
published scientific evidence, involving a very large number of
subjects. At last year's conference of the Parapsychological Association, Dr. Diane Hennacy Powell presented a paper describing a
test with an autistic child, a subject producing results as
spectacular as those produced by Pearce in the 1930's. As discussed here, she showed a
long tape of an experiment with this subject at the conference, one
demonstrating spectacular results. As discussed here, she is currently working on a more
polished film that will present these results to the general public. A preliminary video of previous experiments with this autistic child can be seen here.
No comments:
Post a Comment