There have been many studies of how a brain looks when
particular activities such as thinking or recall occur. Such studies
will typically attempt to find some region of the brain that shows
greater activity when some mental activity occurs. No matter how
slight the evidence is that some particular region is being activated
more strongly, that evidence will be reported and reported as a
“neural correlate” of some activity.
But rather than focusing on a question such as “which brain region
showed the most difference" during some activity, we should look at some more basic and fundamental questions. They
are:
- Does the brain actually look different when it is being scanned while people are doing some mental activity requiring thought or memory recall?
- Does the brain actually become more active while people are doing some mental activity requiring thought or memory recall?
If you were to ask the average person by how much brain
activity increases during some activity such as problem solving or
recall, he might guess 25 or 50 percent, based on all those visuals we
have seen showing brain areas “lighting up” during certain
activities. But as discussed here, such visuals are misleading,
using a visual exaggeration technique that is essentially “lying
with colors.” The key term to get a precise handle of how much
brain activity increases is the term “percent signal change.”
While the visual and auditory cortex regions of the brain (involved
in sensory perception) may increase by much more than 1 percent, this
technical document tells us that “cognitive
effects give signal changes on the order of 1% (and larger in the
visual and auditory cortices).” A similar generalization
is made in this scientific discussion, where it says, based on
previous results, that “most cognitive experiments should show
maximal contrasts of about 1% (except in visual cortex)."
A PhD in neurophysiology states the following:
Those beautiful fMRI scans are misleading, however, because the stark differences they portray are, in fact, minuscule fluctuations that require complex statistical analysis in order to stand out in the pictures. To date, the consensus is that "thinking" has a very minor impact on overall brain metabolism.
A PhD in neurophysiology states the following:
Those beautiful fMRI scans are misleading, however, because the stark differences they portray are, in fact, minuscule fluctuations that require complex statistical analysis in order to stand out in the pictures. To date, the consensus is that "thinking" has a very minor impact on overall brain metabolism.
You can get some exact graphs showing these signal
changes by doing Google searches with phrases such as “neural
correlates of thinking, percent signal change” or “neural
correlates of recollection, percent signal change.” Let's look at
some examples, starting with recollection or memory retrieval.
- This brain scan study was entitled “Working Memory Retrieval: Contributions of the Left Prefrontal Cortex, the Left Posterior Parietal Cortex, and the Hippocampus.” Figure 4 and Figure 5 of the study shows that none of the memory retrievals produced more than a .3 percent signal change, so they all involved signal changes of less than 1 part in 333.
- In this study, brain scans were done during recognition activities, looking for signs of increased brain activity in the hippocampus, a region of the brain often described as some center of brain memory involvement. But the percent signal change is never more than .2 percent, that is, never more than 1 part in 500.
- The paper here is entitled, “Functional-anatomic correlates of remembering and knowing.” It shows a graph showing a percent signal change in the brain during memory retrieval that is no greater than .3 percent, less than 1 part in 300.
- The paper here is entitled “The neural correlates of specific versus general autobiographical memory construction and elaboration.” It shows various graphs showing a percent signal change in the brain during memory retrieval that is no greater than .07 percent, less than 1 part in 1000.
- The paper here is entitled “Neural correlates of true memory, false memory, and deception." It shows various graphs showing a percent signal change during memory retrieval that is no greater than .4 percent, 1 part in 250.
- This paper did a review of 12 other brain scanning studies pertaining to the neural correlates of recollection. Figure 3 of the paper shows an average signal change for different parts of the brain of only about .4 percent, 1 part in 250.
- This paper was entitled “Neural correlates of emotional memories: a review of evidence from brain imaging studies.” We learn from Figure 2 that none of the percent signal changes were greater than .4 percent, 1 part in 250.
- This study was entitled “Sex Differences in the Neural Correlates of Specific and General Autobiographical Memory.” Figure 2 shows that none of the differences in brain activity (for men or women) involved a percent signal change of more than .3 percent or 1 part in 333.
Now let's look at brain scan studies showing brain activity during activities such as thinking, problem solving, and imagination.
- This brain scanning study was entitled “Neural Correlates of Human Virtue Judgment.” Figure 3 shows that none of the regions showed a percent signal change of more than 1 percent, and almost all showed a percent signal change of only about .25 percent (1 part in 400).
- This brain scanning study examined the neural correlates of angry thinking. Table 4 shows that none of the regions studies showed a percent signal change of more than 1.31 percent.
- This brain scanning study was entitled “Neural Activity When People Solve Verbal Problems with Insight.” Figure 2 shows that none of the problem-solving activity produced a percent signal change in the brain of more than .3 percent or about 1 part in 333.
- This study is entitled “Aha!: The Neural Correlates of Verbal Insight Solutions.” Figure 1 shows that none of the brain regions studies had a positive percent signal change of more than .3 percent or about 1 part in 333. Interestingly, one of the brain regions studied had a negative percent signal change of .4 percent that was greater than any of the positive percent signal changes.
- This brain scanning paper is entitled “Neural Correlates of Evaluations of Lying and Truth-Telling in Different Social Contexts.” Figure 3 shows that none of this evaluation activity produced more than a .3 percent signal change in the brain, or about 1 part in 333.
- This brain scanning paper is entitled "In the Zone or Zoning Out? Tracking Behavioral and Neural Fluctuations During Sustained Attention." It tracked brain activity during a mental task requiring attention. The paper's figures show various signal changes in the brain, but none greater than .09 percent, less than 1 part in 1000.
- This brain scanning paper is entitled "Neuronal correlates of familiarity-driven decisions in artificial grammar learning." The paper's figures show various signal changes in the brain, but none greater than 1 percent.
- This brain scanning study is entitled, "Neural correlates of evidence accumulation in a perceptual decision task." The paper's figures show various signal changes in the brain, but none greater than .6 percent, less than 1 part in 150.
- This brain scanning study was entitled, “Neural correlates of the judgment of lying: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study.” We learn from Figure 3 that none of the judgment activity produced a percent signal change in the brain of more than .2 percent or 1 part in 500.
These studies can be summarized like this: during memory recall and thinking or problem solving, the brain does not look any different, and does not work any harder. The tiny differences that show up in these studies are so small they can be characterized as “no significant difference.” You certainly wouldn't claim that an employee was working harder on some day in which you detected that he merely expended a half of one percent more energy, by working two minutes longer on that day. And we shouldn't say the brain is working harder merely because it was detected that some part used only half of one percent more energy, only one part in 200.
As
for whether the brain looks different during thinking or memory recall, based on the
numbers in the studies above, it would seem that someone looking at a
real-time fMRI scanner would be unable to detect a change in
activity when someone was thinking or recalling something. Brain scan studies have the very
bad habit of giving us “lying with color” visuals that may show
some region of the brain highlighted in a bright color, when it
merely displayed a difference of activity of about 1 part in 200. But the
brain would not look that way if you looked at a real-time fMRI scan
of the brain during thinking. Instead, all of the regions would look
the same color (with the exception of visual and auditory cortex
regions that would show a degree of activity corresponding to how much a person was
seeing or hearing). So we can say based on the numbers above that
the brain does not look different when you are thinking or recalling something.
A 1 percent difference cannot even be noticed by the human eye. If I show you two identical-looking photos of a woman, and ask you whether there is any difference, you would be very unlikely to say "yes" if there was merely a 1% difference (such as a width of 200 pixels in one photo and a width of 202 pixels in the second photo). So given the differences discussed above (all 1 percent or less, and most less than half of one percent), it is correct to say that brains do not look different when they are thinking or remembering.
A 1 percent difference cannot even be noticed by the human eye. If I show you two identical-looking photos of a woman, and ask you whether there is any difference, you would be very unlikely to say "yes" if there was merely a 1% difference (such as a width of 200 pixels in one photo and a width of 202 pixels in the second photo). So given the differences discussed above (all 1 percent or less, and most less than half of one percent), it is correct to say that brains do not look different when they are thinking or remembering.
The
relatively tiny variation of the brain during different cognitive
activities is shown by the graph below, which helps to put things in
perspective. The graphed number for the brain (.5 percent) is just
barely visible on the graph.
When you run, the heart gives a very clear sign that it is
involved, and a young man running very fast may have his heart rate
increase by 300%. The pupil of the eye gives a very clear sign that
it is involved with vision, because the pupil of the human eye
changes from a size of 1.5 millimeters to 8 millimeters depending on
how much light is coming into the eye. That's a difference of more
than 500%. But when you think or remember, the brain gives us no clear sign at all that the brain is the source of your thoughts or that memories are being stored or retrieved from the brain. The tiny variations that are seen in brain scans are no greater than we would expect to see from random variations in the brain's blood flow, if the brain did not produce thought and did not store memories. You could find the same level of variation if you were to do fMRI scans of the liver while someone was thinking or remembering.
Concerning glucose levels in the brain, an article in Scientific American tells us that a scientist "remains unconvinced that any one cognitive task measurably changes glucose levels in the brain or blood." According to a scientific paper, "Attempts to measure whole brain changes in blood flow and metabolism during intense mental activity have failed to demonstrate any change." Another paper states this: "Clarke and Sokoloff (1998) remarked that although '[a] common view equates concentrated mental effort with mental work...there appears to be no increased energy utilization by the brain during such processes' (p. 664)."
Concerning glucose levels in the brain, an article in Scientific American tells us that a scientist "remains unconvinced that any one cognitive task measurably changes glucose levels in the brain or blood." According to a scientific paper, "Attempts to measure whole brain changes in blood flow and metabolism during intense mental activity have failed to demonstrate any change." Another paper states this: "Clarke and Sokoloff (1998) remarked that although '[a] common view equates concentrated mental effort with mental work...there appears to be no increased energy utilization by the brain during such processes' (p. 664)."
The reality that the brain does not work harder and does
not look different during thinking or recollection may be shocking to
those who have assumed that the brain is the source of thinking and
the storage place of human memories. But to those who have studied
the numerous reasons for rejecting such dogmas, this reality will
not be surprising at all. To a person who has studied and considered
the lack of any viable theory of permanent memory storage in the
brain (discussed here), and the lack of any viable theory of how a brain could
instantly retrieve memories (discussed here), and the lack of any theory explaining
how a brain could store abstract thoughts as neuron states, it should not be surprising at all to learn that brains do not work harder or look different when you are thinking or recalling. The facts discussed here conflict with the dogmas that brains generate thoughts and store memories. If the brain did such things, we would expect brains to work harder during such activities.
We know for sure that there is a simple type of encoding that goes on in human cells: the encoding needed to implement the genetic code, so that nucleotide base pairs in DNA can be successfully translated into the corresponding amino acids that combinations of the base pairs represent. To accomplish this very simple encoding, the human genome has 620 genes for transfer RNA. But imagine if human brains were to actually encode human experiential and conceptual memories, so that such things were stored in brains. This would be a miracle of encoding many, many times more complicated than the simple encoding that the genetic code involves. Such an encoding would require thousands of dedicated genes in the human genome. But the human genome has been thoroughly mapped, and no such genes have been found. This is an extremely powerful reason for rejecting the dogma that brains store human experiential and conceptual memories.
A good rule to follow is a principle I call "Nobel's Razor." It is the principle that you should believe in scientific work that has won Nobel prizes, but often be skeptical of scientist claims that do not correspond to Nobel prize wins. No scientist has ever won a Nobel prize for any work involving memory, or any work backing up the claim that brains generate thoughts or store memories.
The paper confesses, "All gene expression data are derived from different individuals than the ones that participated in the iEEG study." This means the paper is an absurdity. It is looking for correlations between one set of gene expression data measured in one set of individuals and another set of brain wave data measured in an entirely different set of individuals at a different time. That makes no more sense than trying to look for a correlation between some meal consumption in a 2016 woman's softball team and tooth decay rates in a 2017 male football team.
No comments:
Post a Comment